- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Adding salt to foods and risk of incident depression and anxiety
BMC Medicine volume 23, Article number: 32 (2025)
Abstract
Background
Diet is a well-known determinant of mental health outcomes. However, epidemiologic evidence on salt consumption with the risk of developing depression and anxiety is still very limited. This study aimed to examine the association between adding salt to foods and incident depression and anxiety longitudinally.
Methods
This study used data from 444,787 adults who had never been diagnosed with depression or anxiety at baseline from the UK Biobank, a national community-based cohort from 2006 to 2010. Adding salt to foods was measured using a four-point Likert scale at baseline from a touch-screen questionnaire. The outcomes were incidents of diagnosed depression (F32-F33) and anxiety (F40-F48), defined by the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th Revision codes. Cox proportional hazards models were used to investigate the association between the frequency of adding salt to foods and incident depression and anxiety.
Results
During a mean follow-up period of 14.5 years, 16,319 incidents of depression and 18,959 incidents of anxiety were documented. A higher frequency of adding salt to foods was associated with elevated risk for depression and anxiety. Compared with the group of never/rarely adding salt to foods, the adjusted HRs of incident depression were 1.07 (95% CI: 1.02–1.12), 1.18 (95% CI: 1.10–1.26), and 1.29 (95% CI: 1.18–1.41) across the groups of sometimes, usually, and always, respectively (P trend < 0.001). Participants who reported always adding salt to foods had a 1.17-fold higher risk for developing anxiety (95% CI: 1.07–1.28) compared with those who never/rarely added salt to foods.
Conclusions
A higher frequency of adding salt to foods was independently associated with a higher hazard of depression and anxiety. Interventions such as public awareness campaigns promoting reduced salt consumption may be promising preventative measures to reduce the incidence of depression and anxiety.
Background
Mental disorders are major contributors to the global health-related burden. The Global Burden of Disease Study (GBD) 2021 showed that depressive disorders ranked as the second leading cause of years lived with disability, while anxiety disorders as the sixth [1]. The emergence of the COVID-19 pandemic exacerbated determinants of poor mental health, resulting in a significant increase in the prevalence of both major depressive disorder (27.6%) and anxiety disorders (25.6%) [2]. In 2021, an estimated 332 million (95% UI 298–376) people worldwide were affected by depressive disorders [1]. Given that depressive disorders are rarely detected at their onset and that only a small proportion of individuals receive minimally adequate evidence-based treatment, identifying modifiable factors and developing preventive strategies is crucial [3, 4]. The pressing need for preventative strategies also extends to anxiety disorder, which commonly co-occurs with depressive disorders and exacerbates the disease burden in affected individuals [5, 6].
Diet is a well-known determinant of mental health outcomes. Salt liking, for instance, has been linked to mental disorders. An Australian population survey reported that higher salt liking (pleasantness) was correlated with Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale-21 scores indicative of severe depression and anxiety [7]. Similarly, a biomarker study on depression treatment suggested that higher salt liking was associated with longer disease duration in depressive disorders [8]. Furthermore, studies in healthy populations found that individuals with high trait anxiety had higher salt taste thresholds [9, 10]. In addition to self-reported preference for salty foods, salt consumption can be evaluated using the reported frequency of adding salt to food at the table. Adding salt to foods is a common eating behavior shaped by an individual’s preference for salt and habitual salt intake over the long term [11, 12]. It accounted for 6–20% of total salt consumption in the Western diet and was less likely to be influenced by day-to-day variations [13, 14]. Therefore, adding salt at the table provides a unique assessment to investigate the association between long-term salt taste preference and depression/anxiety. However, despite mounting evidence pointing toward the potential relationship between salt and mental health, very few studies have examined the association of adding salt to foods with depression and anxiety risk.
In the prospective study, we aimed to evaluate the association between the frequency of adding salts to foods and the risk of developing depression and anxiety and further investigate whether genetic predisposition to depression modifies the association.
Methods
Study population
The UK Biobank is a large population-based prospective cohort including over half a million participants aged 37 to 73 years when recruited from 2006 to 2010. Details of the study design have been described elsewhere [15]. For this study, data from 502,422 participants were available. We excluded 56,620 participants with a baseline diagnosis of depression or anxiety, 1007 participants with incomplete data regarding the frequency of adding salt to foods, and eight participants with missing dates of baseline assessment center attendance. A total of 444,787 participants were included in the final analysis. The flowchart of the study population is presented in Additional file 1: Fig. S1. The UK Biobank was approved by the North West Multi-Centre Research Ethics Committee and all participants provided written informed consent.
Exposure assessment
Information on the frequency of adding salt to foods was collected using a touch-screen questionnaire at baseline (2006–2010) [16]. Participants were asked to respond to the question “Do you add salt to your foods? (Do not include salt used in cooking),” and select one answer from four options: (1) never/rarely, (2) sometimes, (3) usually, or (4) always. Those who preferred not to answer were excluded from the final analysis. Additionally, participants were asked, “Have you made any major changes to your diet in the last 5 years?” in the baseline questionnaire. They chose one of the following options: (1) no; (2) yes, because of illness; or (3) yes, because of other reasons.
Outcome identification
Participants diagnosed with depression or anxiety during follow-up were ascertained using the first occurrence of health outcomes in the UK Biobank, which synthesized data from hospital inpatient records, primary care, and death registry data. Incidents of depression and anxiety were defined by International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th Revision (ICD-10) codes F32-F33 and F40-F48, respectively. Follow-up time was calculated from the date of baseline assessment to diagnosis of depression or anxiety, death, or the censoring date (June 5, 2024), whichever came first.
Statistical analysis
Demographic characteristics were presented as mean with SD for continuous variables and as absolute number (percentage) for categorical variables. The chi-square tests for categorical variables and general linear models for continuous variables were performed to compare proportions or means of characteristics between participants with different frequencies of adding salt to foods. Cox proportional hazards models were used to evaluate the association between the frequency of adding salt to foods and the risk of developing depression and anxiety. The proportional hazards assumption was tested by the Kaplan–Meier method and Schoenfeld residuals, and no violation was found. Several potential confounders were adjusted in these models, including age (years, continuous), sex (male or female), race (White or non-White), education level (years, continuous), income (< £18,000, £18,000–£30,999, £31,000–£51,999, £52,000–£100,000, > £100,000, or “do not know”), Townsend deprivation index (TDI, continuous), body mass index (BMI, continuous), smoking status (never, past, or current), alcohol consumption (never, past, or current), healthy diet score (0, 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5) [17, 18], physical activity (high, moderate, or low), sleep behaviors (healthy, intermediate, or poor) [19, 20], diabetes (yes or no), hypertension (yes or no), cholesterol (mmol/L, continuous), cardiovascular disease (CVD, yes or no), spot urinary sodium, and depression polygenic risk score (PRS). The definition of healthy diet score was based on the corresponding median consumption (vegetable intake at least 4 tbsp/day, fruit intake at least 3 pieces/day, fish intake at least twice per week, unprocessed red meat intake no more than twice per week, and processed meat intake less than twice per week). Among the covariates, BMI was calculated using weight (kg) measured with a Tanita BC-418 MA body composition analyzer and height (m) measured with a Seca 202 device. Biochemistry markers, including cholesterol levels, were assayed using the AU5800 analytical platform (Beckman Coulter). Sociodemographic factors, dietary intake, and lifestyle factors were self-reported. Baseline health conditions, including diabetes, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease, were identified through hospital inpatient records, primary care data, death registries, and self-reported questionnaires. Detailed assessments for confounders are described in the Additional file 1.
We hypothesized that the association between the frequency of adding salt to foods and incidents of depression or anxiety may be modified by sex (female or male), age (< 60 or ≥ 60 years), TDI (quintiles 1, quintiles 2–4, quintile 5), education level (< 16 years or ≥ 16 years), BMI (18.5–25, 25–30, ≥ 30 kg/m2), physical activity (high, moderate, or low), 24-h weighted average noise (≤ median or > median), and depression PRS (tertile 1, tertile 2, or tertile 3). Stratified analyses with hazard ratios (HRs) and 95%CIsestimated within the strata were conducted. Multiplicative interaction was assessed by adding interaction terms to the Cox models.
Several sensitivity analyses were performed to verify the robustness of the results. First, to minimize the influence of reverse causation, we excluded depression and anxiety cases that occurred in the first year of follow-up. Second, to control for the impact of diet change on the frequency of adding salt to foods, we excluded participants who had changed their diet in the last 5 years due to illness or other reasons. Third, we performed a sensitivity analysis by further adjusting for total energy intake in participants with available data. Fourth, we excluded individuals with hypertension at baseline. Fifth, we further adjusted the estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR). Sixth, we adjusted for education level as a categorical variable (university or nonuniversity) rather than a continuous one. Seventh, we conducted an additional sensitivity analysis, adjusting for further covariates, using “living with a spouse or partner” as an indicator of marital status and “cancer history of father, mother, and siblings” as an indicator of family members’ health status. Finally, we conducted another sensitivity analysis by adjusting BMI PRS instead of BMI.
All statistical analyses were conducted using StataMP 17 and R version 4.0.3. All statistical tests were two-sided, and P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. A Bonferroni-corrected 2-sided P value of 0.006 was used as the significance threshold for the eight interaction analyses due to multiple comparisons.
Results
Baseline characteristics of participants according to the frequency of adding salt to foods
Of the participants, 55.7% reported never/rarely adding salt to foods, 28.0% added salt sometimes, 11.6% usually, and 4.7% always (Table 1). Compared with participants with a lower frequency of adding salt to foods, participants with a higher frequency were more likely to be men, non-white, and to have higher TDI and BMI; were less likely to adopt a healthy lifestyle (never smoking, high physical activity, healthy diet); and had a lower prevalence of hypertension but a higher prevalence of CVD.
Association between the frequency of adding salt to foods and depression risk
During a mean follow-up of 14.5 years, 16,319 incident cases of depression were documented. After adjustment for age, sex, race, education, income, TDI, BMI, smoking, alcohol consumption, physical activity, healthy diet score, hypertension, diabetes, cholesterol, CVD, healthy sleep behaviors, spot urinary sodium, and depression PRS, the hazard of depression increased monotonously with increasing frequency of adding salts to foods. The adjusted HRs were 1.00 (reference), 1.07 (95% CI: 1.02–1.12), 1.18 (95% CI: 1.10–1.26), and 1.29 (95% CI: 1.18–1.41) across the groups never/rarely, sometimes, usually, and always, respectively (P trend < 0.001, Table 2).
Association between the frequency of adding salt to foods and anxiety risk
For anxiety, 18,959 incidents were identified during a total of 6,426,051 person-years’ follow-up. Participants who always added salt to foods were at significantly higher risk for anxiety compared to those who never/rarely added salt (HR 1.17, 95% CI: 1.07–1.28) (Table 2).
Subgroup analysis
No significant multiplicative interaction was observed between self-reported frequency of adding salt to foods and each confounder (e.g., age, sex, BMI, education level, TDI, noise, physical activity, and depression PRS) on developing depression and anxiety, as all P values for interaction were greater than the Bonferroni-adjusted threshold of 0.006 (Table 3).
Joint association of the frequency of adding salt to foods and depression prs with risk of depression and anxiety
Participants were classified into joint categories based on the frequency of adding salt to foods and tertiles of depression PRS, with those who never/rarely added salt to foods and were in the lowest depression PRS tertile as the reference (Fig. 1). Participants with the highest frequency of adding salt to foods (always) and the highest depression PRS tertile were at the greatest risk of incident depression (HR 1.78, 95% CI:1.54–2.05) and anxiety (HR 1.30, 95% CI:1.12–1.51). The association between adding salt to foods and the risk of depression appeared to be consistent and significant across all three depression PRS categories.
Sensitivity analysis
The results did not change appreciably after further excluding incidents that occurred during the first year of follow-up, excluding participants with hypertension at baseline, or those who had changed their diet in the last 5 years because of illness or other reasons. The findings remained robust after adjusting for education level as a categorical variable, using BMI PRS instead of BMI, and further adjusting for total energy intake, eGFR, living with a spouse or partner, and family cancer history (Additional file 1: Table S1).
Discussion
In this large prospective study, we observed that a higher frequency of adding salt to foods was associated with an increased risk of incident depression and anxiety, independent of socioeconomic status, lifestyle factors, pre-existing diseases, and genetic susceptibility to depression. Furthermore, the association showed a robust dose-dependent increase.
Our findings on the adverse impact of frequently adding salt to foods with depression risk are consistent with previous studies. For instance, data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), including more than 10,000 participants [21], showed that individuals with depression were more likely to add salt to their food. The salt CONNtrol trial demonstrated that salt restriction entailed improvement in depressive symptoms [22].
Several mechanisms could possibly explain the associations between the higher frequency of adding salt to foods and increased risks of depression and anxiety. First, sodium ions play a crucial role in terminating synaptic transmission and recycling neurotransmitters for reuse [23,24,25]. Disruptions of sodium ion balance can affect the functioning of neurotransmitters (e.g., dopamine, noradrenaline, and 5-hydroxytryptamine) vital for locomotion, autonomic function, hormone secretion, and complex behaviors, which are all associated with emotions, and the development of mood disorders [26,27,28,29,30]. Second, high salt consumption hyperactivates the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and amplifies stress response [31, 32]. Stress-induced hyperactivity of the HPA axis leads to excessive glucocorticoid release, which continuously and intensely stimulates the glucocorticoid receptors of neurons in the hippocampus and other brain regions, causing symptoms such as neuronal atrophy, loss of dendritic spines, neurogenesis damage, and reduced synthesis of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [33,34,35,36,37,38]. These neurobiological processes are commonly observed among patients with depression and anxiety [39,40,41]. Third, high salt intake is associated with disturbance in the gut microbiota, which could induce the production of inflammatory cytokines and cause glial cell changes by passing through the blood–brain barrier, potentially contributing to depression and anxiety [42,43,44,45,46,47,48].
To our knowledge, this is the first large-scale prospective cohort study with the longest follow-up duration that highlights the relationship between the frequency of adding salt to foods and incident depression and anxiety. Moreover, the incident of depression and anxiety was recorded using linkage data from hospital inpatient records, primary care records, and death registries. This method avoids information bias and increases the accuracy and reliability of the records.
Limitations
Several limitations should be acknowledged. First, the self-reported nature of adding salt frequency, sociodemographic factors, and lifestyle factors could be subject to recall bias and misclassification. Second, despite our attempt to exclude individuals who had changed their diet in the past 5 years due to illnesses or other reasons, other lifestyle changes were likely to occur during the long follow-up period. Although the sensitivity analysis indicated the robustness of the principal findings, future studies may consider examining the modifying effect of other lifestyle changes on this relationship. Third, while we adjusted for a wide range of potential confounders, residual confounding cannot be entirely ruled out. Fourth, volunteer selection bias may exist in our study because the response rate at baseline in the UK Biobank was only 5.5% [49]. However, many associations observed in other studies could be replicated in the UK Biobank, suggesting that selection bias, if existent in this study, is not greater than that in others [50]. Fifth, another potential limitation of this study is the possibility of response style bias, which occurs when participants tend to respond to questionnaire items in a systematic manner that may not accurately reflect their true behaviors or experiences [51]. Finally, the possibility of reverse causality cannot be eliminated. While evidence from animal models provided the physiologic underpinning of reverse causality, its assessment in humans remains challenging [52,53,54]. Future large-scale clinical trials on salt reduction, assessing changes in depressive and anxiety symptoms, are needed to obtain higher-level evidence to support the findings of this study.
Conclusions
In conclusion, this study revealed a significant association between a higher frequency of adding salt to foods and increased risks of incident depression and anxiety. These findings suggest that adding salt to foods could be a potential target for intervention strategies aimed at reducing incidence of these mental disorders. Further multiple population-representative research would be warranted to demonstrate generalizability.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the UK Biobank. Access to the UK Biobank resource is available via an approved application (http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk). Analytic codes are available upon reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- GBD:
-
Global Burden of Disease
- ICD-10:
-
International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th Revision
- TDI:
-
Townsend deprivation index
- BMI:
-
Body mass index
- CVD:
-
Cardiovascular disease
- PRS:
-
Polygenic risk score
- eGFR:
-
Estimated glomerular filtration rate
- NHANES:
-
National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey
- HPA:
-
Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
- BDNF:
-
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor
References
GBD 2021 Diseases and Injuries Collaborators. Global incidence, prevalence, years lived with disability (YLDs), disability-adjusted life-years (DALYs), and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 371 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories and 811 subnational locations, 1990–2021: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2021. Lancet. 2024;403(10440):2133–61.
COVID-19 Mental Disorders Collaborators. Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet. 2021;398(10312):1700–12.
GBD 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators. Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry. 2022;9(2):137–50.
Patel V, Saxena S, Lund C, Thornicroft G, Baingana F, Bolton P, et al. The Lancet Commission on global mental health and sustainable development. Lancet. 2018;392(10157):1553–98.
Kaufman J, Charney D. Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety. 2000;12(Suppl 1):69–76.
Penninx BW, Pine DS, Holmes EA, Reif A. Anxiety disorders. Lancet. 2021;397(10277):914–27.
Ferraris C, Scarlett CJ, Bucher T, Beckett EL. Liking of salt is associated with depression, anxiety, and stress. Chem Senses. 2023;48:bjad038.
Buttner M, Jezova D, Greene B, Konrad C, Kircher T, Murck H. Target-based biomarker selection - mineralocorticoid receptor-related biomarkers and treatment outcome in major depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;66–67:24–37.
Heath TP, Melichar JK, Nutt DJ, Donaldson LF. Human taste thresholds are modulated by serotonin and noradrenaline. J Neurosci. 2006;26(49):12664–71.
Ileri-Gurel E, Pehlivanoglu B, Dogan M. Effect of acute stress on taste perception: in relation with baseline anxiety level and body weight. Chem Senses. 2013;38(1):27–34.
Van der Veen JE, De Graaf C, Van Dis SJ, Van Staveren WA. Determinants of salt use in cooked meals in The Netherlands: attitudes and practices of food preparers. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1999;53(5):388–94.
Quader ZS, Zhao L, Harnack LJ, Gardner CD, Shikany JM, Steffen LM, et al. Self-reported measures of discretionary salt use accurately estimated sodium intake overall but not in certain subgroups of US adults from 3 geographic regions in the Salt Sources Study. J Nutr. 2019;149(9):1623–32.
Mattes RD, Donnelly D. Relative contributions of dietary sodium sources. J Am Coll Nutr. 1991;10(4):383–93.
Sutherland J, Edwards P, Shankar B, Dangour AD. Fewer adults add salt at the table after initiation of a national salt campaign in the UK: a repeated cross-sectional analysis. Br J Nutr. 2013;110(3):552–8.
Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, Beral V, Burton P, Danesh J, et al. UK Biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. Plos Med. 2015;12(3):e1001779.
Bradbury KE, Young HJ, Guo W, Key TJ. Dietary assessment in UK Biobank: an evaluation of the performance of the touchscreen dietary questionnaire. J Nutr Sci. 2018;7: e6.
Lloyd-Jones DM, Hong Y, Labarthe D, Mozaffarian D, Appel LJ, Van Horn L, et al. Defining and setting national goals for cardiovascular health promotion and disease reduction: the American Heart Association’s strategic Impact Goal through 2020 and beyond. Circulation. 2010;121(4):586–613.
Pazoki R, Dehghan A, Evangelou E, Warren H, Gao H, Caulfield M, et al. Genetic predisposition to high blood pressure and lifestyle factors: associations with midlife blood pressure levels and cardiovascular events. Circulation. 2018;137(7):653–61.
Fan M, Sun D, Zhou T, Heianza Y, Lv J, Li L, et al. Sleep patterns, genetic susceptibility, and incident cardiovascular disease: a prospective study of 385 292 UK biobank participants. Eur Heart J. 2020;41(11):1182–9.
Wang M, Zhou T, Li X, Ma H, Liang Z, Fonseca VA, et al. Baseline vitamin D status, sleep patterns, and the risk of incident type 2 diabetes in data from the UK Biobank study. Diabetes Care. 2020;43(11):2776–84.
Goldstein P, Leshem M. Dietary sodium, added salt, and serum sodium associations with growth and depression in the US general population. Appetite. 2014;79:83–90.
Schneider H, Sarkis AL, Sturm L, Britz V, Lechner A, Potzel AL, et al. Moderate dietary salt restriction improves blood pressure and mental well-being in patients with primary aldosteronism: the salt CONNtrol trial. J Intern Med. 2023;294(1):47–57.
Worrall DM, Williams DC. Sodium ion-dependent transporters for neurotransmitters: a review of recent developments. Biochem J. 1994;297(Pt 3):425–36.
Kanner BI. Sodium-coupled neurotransmitter transport: structure, function and regulation. J Exp Biol. 1994;196:237–49.
Shi L, Quick M, Zhao Y, Weinstein H, Javitch JA. The mechanism of a neurotransmitter:sodium symporter–inward release of Na+ and substrate is triggered by substrate in a second binding site. Mol Cell. 2008;30(6):667–77.
Martin H, Bullich S, Martinat M, Chataigner M, Di Miceli M, Simon V, et al. Insulin modulates emotional behavior through a serotonin-dependent mechanism. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;29(6):1610–9.
Ogawa S, Fujii T, Koga N, Hori H, Teraishi T, Hattori K, et al. Plasma L-tryptophan concentration in major depressive disorder: new data and meta-analysis. J Clin Psychiatry. 2014;75(9):e906–15.
Bekhbat M, Li Z, Mehta ND, Treadway MT, Lucido MJ, Woolwine BJ, et al. Functional connectivity in reward circuitry and symptoms of anhedonia as therapeutic targets in depression with high inflammation: evidence from a dopamine challenge study. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;27(10):4113–21.
Cui Y, Yang Y, Ni Z, Dong Y, Cai G, Foncelle A, et al. Astroglial Kir4.1 in the lateral habenula drives neuronal bursts in depression. Nature. 2018;554(7692):323–7.
Cui L, Li S, Wang S, Wu X, Liu Y, Yu W, et al. Major depressive disorder: hypothesis, mechanism, prevention and treatment. Signal Transduct Target Ther. 2024;9(1):30.
Costello HM, Krilis G, Grenier C, Severs D, Czopek A, Ivy JR, et al. High salt intake activates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, amplifies the stress response, and alters tissue glucocorticoid exposure in mice. Cardiovasc Res. 2023;119(8):1740–50.
Wang Y, Liu L, Gu JH, Wang CN, Guan W, Liu Y, et al. Salt-inducible kinase 1-CREB-regulated transcription coactivator 1 signalling in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus plays a role in depression by regulating the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;29(6):1660–70.
Herbert J, Goodyer IM, Grossman AB, Hastings MH, De Kloet ER, Lightman SL, et al. Do corticosteroids damage the brain? J Neuroendocrinol. 2006;18(6):393–411.
Hall BS, Moda RN, Liston C. Glucocorticoid mechanisms of functional connectivity changes in stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders. Neurobiol Stress. 2015;1:174–83.
Barfield ET, Gourley SL. Prefrontal cortical trkB, glucocorticoids, and their interactions in stress and developmental contexts. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;95:535–58.
Boda E. Myelin and oligodendrocyte lineage cell dysfunctions: new players in the etiology and treatment of depression and stress-related disorders. Eur J Neurosci. 2021;53(1):281–97.
Cheng JD, de Vellis J. Oligodendrocytes as glucocorticoids target cells: functional analysis of the glycerol phosphate dehydrogenase gene. J Neurosci Res. 2000;59(3):436–45.
Chen ZY, Jing D, Bath KG, Ieraci A, Khan T, Siao CJ, et al. Genetic variant BDNF (Val66Met) polymorphism alters anxiety-related behavior. Science. 2006;314(5796):140–3.
Keller J, Gomez R, Williams G, Lembke A, Lazzeroni L, Murphy GM Jr, et al. HPA axis in major depression: cortisol, clinical symptomatology and genetic variation predict cognition. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22(4):527–36.
Mantella RC, Butters MA, Amico JA, Mazumdar S, Rollman BL, Begley AE, et al. Salivary cortisol is associated with diagnosis and severity of late-life generalized anxiety disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(6):773–81.
Martin EI, Ressler KJ, Binder E, Nemeroff CB. The neurobiology of anxiety disorders: brain imaging, genetics, and psychoneuroendocrinology. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2009;32(3):549–75.
Sampson TR, Debelius JW, Thron T, Janssen S, Shastri GG, Ilhan ZE, et al. Gut microbiota regulate motor deficits and neuroinflammation in a model of Parkinson’s disease. Cell. 2016;167(6):1469-80 e12.
Fan L, Peng Y, Wang J, Ma P, Zhao L, Li X. Total glycosides from stems of Cistanche tubulosa alleviate depression-like behaviors: bidirectional interaction of the phytochemicals and gut microbiota. Phytomedicine. 2021;83: 153471.
Zhang JC, Yao W, Dong C, Yang C, Ren Q, Ma M, et al. Blockade of interleukin-6 receptor in the periphery promotes rapid and sustained antidepressant actions: a possible role of gut-microbiota-brain axis. Transl Psychiatry. 2017;7(5): e1138.
Pan Y, Chen XY, Zhang QY, Kong LD. Microglial NLRP3 inflammasome activation mediates IL-1beta-related inflammation in prefrontal cortex of depressive rats. Brain Behav Immun. 2014;41:90–100.
Wang YL, Han QQ, Gong WQ, Pan DH, Wang LZ, Hu W, et al. Microglial activation mediates chronic mild stress-induced depressive- and anxiety-like behavior in adult rats. J Neuroinflammation. 2018;15(1):21.
McKim DB, Weber MD, Niraula A, Sawicki CM, Liu X, Jarrett BL, et al. Microglial recruitment of IL-1beta-producing monocytes to brain endothelium causes stress-induced anxiety. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23(6):1421–31.
Wilck N, Matus MG, Kearney SM, Olesen SW, Forslund K, Bartolomaeus H, et al. Salt-responsive gut commensal modulates T(H)17 axis and disease. Nature. 2017;551(7682):585–9.
Allen N, Sudlow C, Downey P, Peakman T, Danesh J, Elliott P, et al. UK Biobank: current status and what it means for epidemiology. Health Policy and Techn. 2012;1(3):123–6.
Batty GD, Gale CR, Kivimäki M, Deary IJ, Bell S. Comparison of risk factor associations in UK Biobank against representative, general population based studies with conventional response rates: prospective cohort study and individual participant meta-analysis. Bmj-Brit Med J. 2020;368:m131.
Van Vaerenbergh Y, Thomas TD. Response styles in survey research: a literature review of antecedents, consequences, and remedies. Int J Public Opin Res. 2013;25(2):195–217.
Leshem M. Low dietary sodium is anxiogenic in rats. Physiol Behav. 2011;103(5):453–8.
Weisinger RS, Blair-West JR, Burns P, Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Purcell B, et al. The inhibitory effect of hormones associated with stress on Na appetite of sheep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(6):2922–7.
Murck H, Lehr L, Jezova D. A viewpoint on aldosterone and BMI related brain morphology in relation to treatment outcome in patients with major depression. J Neuroendocrinol. 2023;35(2):e13219.
Acknowledgements
This research was conducted using the UK Biobank resources under application 66137. We thank the participants for sharing their health related information. The analysis of this study was supported by the Center for Computational Science and Engineering at Southern University of Science and Technology.
Funding
This research was supported by Capital’s Funds for Health Improvement and Research (2024–4-2129), Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (Z221100007422010), and Beijing High level Public Health Professionals Training Plan (xuekegugan-01–12).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
TW, MW, JH, and WW conceptualized the study. WW and MW analyzed the data. WW drafted the manuscript. MW, WW, FL, LF, and TW contributed to discussion. TW, MW, XC, FL, and JH reviewed and edited the manuscript. WW, TW, MW, XC, FL, LF, and JH are the guarantors of this work and, as such, had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The generic ethical approval was obtained by UK Biobank from the NHS National Research Ethics Service (approval letter dated June 17th 2011, Ref 11/NW/0382). All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the UK Biobank.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
12916_2025_3865_MOESM1_ESM.docx
Additional file 1: S1. Supplementary Methods. Fig. S1. Provides the participant flow chart. Table S1. Results of sensitivity analyses.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Wang, W., Chang, X., Lin, F. et al. Adding salt to foods and risk of incident depression and anxiety. BMC Med 23, 32 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12916-025-03865-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12916-025-03865-x