Skip to main content

Disparities in neighborhood food environment and cognitive decline among US older adults: a cohort study

Abstract

Background

Disparities in neighborhood food environments in the United States, attributed to numerous complex economic, social, and political factors, likely to contribute to disparities in access to healthy food and cognitive function in older adults. However, the role of food environment in cognitive function is not well understood. Accordingly, this study examined the association of residing a low food access and low-income neighborhood with changes in cognitive function among older adults in urban areas.

Methods

This is a cohort study leveraging existing datasets. The 2010 Food Access Research Atlas data was linked to the 2011–2021 National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS). A total of 4768 urban-dwelling older adults aged 65 years and older were included in this analysis. Total cognitive function (range: 0–33) was assessed through tests of orientation, executive function, immediate memory, and delayed memory. An unhealthy food environment was defined as residing in census tracts with both low access to healthy food stores and low income. Survey-weighted mixed-effects models were fitted, adjusting for individual- and area-level covariates.

Results

The mean age of participants was 77.1 years (SD = 7.6), and 2779 were women (weighted % = 56.7). A total of 1238 participants (weighted % = 9.9%) were racialized as Black, 365 (weighted % = 9.1%) racialized as Latinx, and 3165 (weighted % = 81.1%) racialized as White. In adjusted models, older urban- and community-dwelling adults living in neighborhoods with low access and low income had faster annual cognitive decline than their peers (β = − 0.19; 95% CI = − 0.32, − 0.05).

Conclusions

Living in neighborhoods with both low food access and low income may be a risk factor for accelerated cognitive decline among urban-dwelling older adults and contribute to widening disparities in healthy food access and cognitive decline.

Peer Review reports

Background

A neighborhood food environment refers to the areas where people acquire and consume food [1, 2], including food stores, food retail establishments, community gardens, and food assistance programs. Disparities in the neighborhood food environments in the United States (US) are attributed to numerous complex economic, social, and political factors [3]. In addition to neoliberal economic policies—characterized by deregulation, privatization, and a profit-driven approach—and changes in food supply chains, demographic shifts occurred in large US cities between the 1970s and 1980s where affluent White households moved from urban areas to outlying suburban areas, leading to the closures of stores in the inner cities and the relocation of grocery stores in the suburban [4]. Ongoing disinvestment related to residential segregation or redlining (i.e., historic discriminatory housing practice) in low-income communities lacking purchasing power has restricted food access, which is exacerbated in neighborhoods with residents racialized as Black and Latinx [4,5,6]. Consequently, low-income areas, such as neighborhoods with residents racialized as Black and Latinx, tend to have fewer supermarkets [4, 7]. Food stores in these areas often offer fewer healthy food options [8, 9], low-quality fresh produce [10], and higher prices for the same items than in higher-income areas when healthier items are available [11]. Additionally, low-income neighborhoods tend to have more unhealthy food outlets, such as fast-food restaurants and convenience stores selling predominantly processed, energy-dense foods [12,13,14,15].

The food environment is a social determinant of health that not only influences nutrition insecurity but health outcomes [5] like cognitive function, which is critical for health and well-being in later life. Rapid cognitive decline or decline in multiple cognitive domains can be an indication of a preclinical stage of dementia [16]. Cognitive decline interferes with daily activities necessary for independent and healthy living in older adults, including driving [17], financial decision-making [18], and diabetes self-management [19]. Residing in unhealthy food environment is associated with poorer dietary quality [20,21,22,23] and food insecurity [24, 25], which are associated with accelerated cognitive decline and risk factors for dementia like obesity, diabetes, and hypertension [26,27,28,29,30,31]. Living far away from grocery stores is challenging for older adults, as they are more likely to experience dementia, diminished physical function, visual impairment, social isolation, and poverty, all of which can make accessing food stores even more difficult. However, the relationship between food environment and cognitive decline has largely been overlooked in US older adults.

The food environment encompasses various kinds of food resources, each having potentially different impacts on cognitive function, but the association of access to healthy food stores with cognitive decline in US older adults is not well understood. For example, a mixed-methods study in Minneapolis found that urban and suburban community-dwelling older adults residing in neighborhoods with more eateries (e.g., coffee shops and fast-food restaurants) had better cognitive function than their peers [32]. However, this type of food resources may confer a social benefit rather than a nutritional benefit and another study accounting for other neighborhood characteristics found opposite results [33]. A national cohort study found that residing in neighborhoods with more grocery stores was not associated with cognitive function among individuals aged 45 or older, racialized as Black and White, in metropolitan areas [33]. It is important to include both physical food access and economic conditions of the neighborhood in measuring the food environment to better capture the complex underlying contextual features. However, this measure of the food environment has not been tested in relation to cognitive decline in older adults.

To address this research gap, the current study examined the neighborhood food environments operationalized as low access to healthy food stores and low income in relation to cognitive function, leveraging data from a nationally representative sample of Medicare beneficiaries aged 65 and older living in urban communities in the US.

Methods

Study design and participants

This study employed a cohort study design by linking 2010 Food Access Research Atlas data [34] to the 2011–2021 National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) [35]. NHATS provides data on a nationally representative cohort of Medicare beneficiaries aged 65 and older, initially recruited in 2011 and replenished in 2015 using a stratified three-stage sampling approach [36]. We included the 2011 cohort of community-dwelling participants who did not have dementia diagnosis at study enrollment and lived in urban neighborhoods (n = 5125). Urban neighborhoods were defined based on the US Census Bureau’s classification, where a census tract is considered urban if its geographic centroid encompasses a population of more than 2500 people [37]. NHATS conducted annual at-home interviews from 2011 to 2019, over the phone in 2020, and both in person and via phone in 2021 due to coronavirus disease of 2019 [38]. We excluded participants from rural areas since food environment in rural areas are qualitatively different from those in urban areas [39], and only few people (n = 91) lived in low-access and low-income neighborhoods, which limited statistical power. Additionally, fewer than 12 participants reported being racialized differently than the survey categories of Black, Latinx, or White that live in areas with low access and low income. To protect confidentiality and avoid combining the racialized groups into one group, we excluded participants who reported being racialized differently than the survey categories (n = 155). Lastly, we excluded participants missing study variables (n = 202, 4.06%) leaving a sample size of 4768 participants for this analysis (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of eligible participants over the study period (2011–2021)

Outcomes variables

Total cognitive function score was measured annually using domain-specific cognitive performance tests (i.e., orientation, executive function, immediate memory, and delayed memory). Orientation was assessed by querying date, month, year, and day of week and naming president and vice president (range 0 to 8). A clock drawing test was conducted to evaluate executive function, scored from 0 signifying not recognizable as a clock to 5 indicating an accurate representation of a clock [38, 40]. In 2020, materials were mailed to participants and a clock drawing test was administered over the phone. The materials were returned by mail for scoring. Immediate and delayed memory were tested by 10-word list memory and recall tests [41]. All domain-specific cognitive assessment scores were summed to create total cognitive function score (range 0 to 33), where higher scores indicate better cognitive function [42]. Previous literature suggested that there were no significant differences in total cognitive function score by survey mode [43, 44].

Exposure variable

As in prior work, census tracts were classified as being an unhealthy food environment if they had both low food access and low income [45,46,47]. The Food Access Research Atlas by the US Department of Agriculture (USDA) defined low food access as a census tract where a significant number (i.e., more than 500 people or 33%) of population lived greater than 1 mile from the nearest affordable and healthy food resources including supermarkets, supercenters, or large grocery stores [37]. A census tract was considered as a low-income area if the poverty rate was greater than 20% or median family income was less than 80% of the state-wide or surrounding metropolitan area median family income [37]. Low food access and low income often co-occur in neighborhoods, creating compounded barriers to consistently and safely accessing nutritious food [48]. This overlapping of low food access and low income makes it crucial to evaluate both factors together to better predict their joint impact on health.

Confounders

Potential confounders included individual- and area-level factors that are likely to be associated with living in area with low access and low income as well as cognitive function. These factors were measured at baseline using data from the NHATS dataset. Individual-level factors included age in year, gender (male, female), racialized group (Black, Latinx, White), educational attainment (below high school, high school graduate, above high school), log-transformed income, and living arrangement (alone, with partner, with partner and others, with others). Area-level factors included US census regions (Northeast, Midwest, South, and West) and street disorder at baseline. To assess street disorder, an interviewer rated three items from 1 (none) to 4 (a lot) and averaged the scores across items: amount of litter or broken glass on streets, graffiti on buildings, and vacant stores of houses around participants’ home [49]. Year since study enrollment (wave) was included as a time-varying variable. Street disorder was included as a confounder because low-income neighborhoods tend to have more features of disorder, such as graffiti, broken windows, and litter [50]. Additionally, neighborhood disorder has been associated with poorer cognitive function [51, 52].

Statistical analysis

Less than 5% of observations were missing for all study variables except income (Fig. 1). The NHATS provided imputed values for total income: 13% of the sample had income values imputed within a reported bracketed value and 31% of the sample had an imputed value based on sources of income, the amount of income from each source, and auxiliary variables such as home ownership, labor force status, and educational attainment [53]. Descriptive statistics were expressed as count and percentage or mean and standard deviation and were compared for participants living in low-access and low-income neighborhoods to those not at baseline using weighted Pearson \(\chi\) 2 statistics or bivariate linear regression model.

Mixed-effect models with random intercepts (individual) and slopes (time) were used to test the association between living in neighborhood with low access and low-income and cognitive change. Study year (wave) was used for a time metric. All models included main effect terms of study year and an indicator for living in a neighborhood with low access and low-income and their interaction term. In these models, the hypothesized association between living in neighborhood with low access and low-income and cognitive change over time was tested using the interaction term between year with low access and low-income neighborhood. The coefficient for year represented annual change in cognitive function score and the coefficient for living in a neighborhood with low access and low-income estimated the association between living in the unhealthy food environment with baseline total cognitive function score. Adjusted models controlled for baseline individual- and area-level factors. Additionally, we included interactions between time with racialized group and gender, as these factors are known to be associated with different cognitive trajectories over time [54, 55]. Analytic weights at each round were applied to account for nonresponse and the complex sampling design. Marginal means of total cognitive score over the study period were estimated from the fully adjusted mixed-effect models and used to construct a plot of total cognitive score trajectories by living in a neighborhood with low access and low-income compared to not living in these neighborhoods. Data analyses occurred between June 13, 2023 and April 30, 2024. Statistical significance was considered at P value of less than 0.05 (2-sided test). Analyses were conducted using Stata, version 16.1 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX).

Results

Participants and neighborhood characteristics

Participants included in this analysis (N = 4768) completed an average of 5.36 research visits (SD = 3.9). At baseline, 13.1% of participants (n = 717) lived in a neighborhood with low access and low-income (Table 1). The mean age of participants was 77.1 years (SD = 7.6) and 2779 were women (weighted % = 56.7). A total of 1238 participants (weighted % = 9.9%) were racialized as Black, 365 (weighted % = 9.1%) were racialized as Latinx, and 3165 (weighted % = 81.1%) were racialized as White. Participants who were racialized as Black or Latinx, as well as those with lower income and educational attainment, were more likely to live in neighborhoods with low food access and low-income. Total cognitive function score at baseline was lower among individuals living in low food access and low-income neighborhoods compared to those not living in such neighborhoods (mean = 15.7, SD = 5.4 vs. mean = 17.3, SD = 5.2, respectively).

Table 1 Characteristics of participants at study enrollment by neighborhood food environment, National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) (N = 4768)

Living in neighborhoods with both low food access and low income was also associated with residing on streets with higher street disorder scores and with living in the Midwest and South census regions (Table 1). Additionally, low-income neighborhoods were more likely to be classified as low food access neighborhoods (Additional file 1: Table 1).

Living in neighborhoods with low food access and low income and cognitive change

Living in a neighborhood with low access and low-income was not associated with baseline total cognitive function score in fully adjusted model (β = 0.58; 95% CI = − 0.35, 1.50) (Table 2). However, residing in a neighborhood with low access and low-income was associated with a faster decline in total cognitive scores (β = − 0.19; 95% CI = − 0.32, − 0.05). As shown in Fig. 2, the baseline total cognitive function score was slightly higher among individuals not living in low-access and low-income neighborhoods compared to their peers. However, those residing in such neighborhoods experienced a faster decline in total cognitive function over time, leading to a crossover in the cognitive function score trajectories of the two groups.

Table 2 Association between living in neighborhoods with low food access and low income and cognitive function among US urban community-dwelling older adults (N = 4768)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Marginal predicted mean of total cognition over the study period comparing participants residing in neighborhoods with low food access and low income to those not living in those neighborhoods

We conducted an additional analysis to test the additive effects by creating a categorical variable. The reference group (value = 0) includes participants living in neighborhoods that were neither low-income nor low food access. A value of 1 represents living in a low-income neighborhood but not a low food access neighborhood. A value of 2 indicates living in a low food access neighborhood but not a low-income neighborhood. A value of 3 represents living in a neighborhood classified as both low-income and low food access. The findings were consistent with the results above: individuals residing in low access and low-income neighborhoods experienced a faster decline in total cognitive function compared to those living in neighborhoods that were neither low access nor low-income (β = − 0.19; 95% CI = − 0.35, − 0.02) (Additional file 1: Table 2).

Furthermore, we conducted a multiplicative analysis using separate binary variables for low-income and low food access. The findings were consistent with our primary analyses: residing in neighborhoods characterized by both low-income and low food access was associated with a faster decline in cognitive function over time (β = − 0.19; 95% CI = − 0.38, − 0.00) (Additional file 1: Table 3).

Discussion

This study found that living in a neighborhood with low food access and low-income is associated with accelerated cognitive decline among urban community-dwelling older adults, even after adjusting for individual socioeconomic factors such as income and education. Even though we observed a small decline (0.19 points per year on a scale ranging from 0 to 33), this change is still meaningful. This is because the steeper rate of cognitive decline observed in individuals living in poor food environments, compared to those in better-resourced areas for healthy and affordable food purchasing, is not a part of normal biological aging. Rather, it likely reflects a process of accelerated cognitive decline related to structural determinants that disproportionately affect low income and racially and ethnically minoritized communities, further exacerbating existing disparities in cognitive health. This study leveraged data collected over 10 years from an ongoing nationally representative cohort of Medicare beneficiaries ages 65 and older. These findings build on prior studies by measuring urban neighborhood food environments based on having both low food access and low-income, highlighting this measure as an important indicator of health among community-dwelling older adults. This contributes to a better understanding of the interconnectedness of neighborhood lo- income status and low neighborhood food access—conditions that often overlap and co-occur—and its relationship to cognitive function among community-dwelling older adults. Furthermore, these results contribute to the literature by suggesting that the food environment is a structural risk factor for accelerated cognitive decline among older adults living in urban areas.

The association between residing in areas with low food access and low-income and cognitive decline among urban-dwelling older adults can be explained by several potential mechanisms, including poor dietary quality, food insecurity, and stress-related pathways. Limited access to healthy food is associated with poor diet quality [56] and food insecurity [57], both of which have been linked to cognitive decline [31, 58,59,60]. The imposed poor or insufficient diets can also contribute to greater risk for diet-related diseases such as obesity, diabetes, and hypertension [61, 62], which are risk factors for cognitive decline [63,64,65]. Additionally, stress or depression from living in a disadvantaged neighborhood [66] may contribute to cognitive decline through systemic inflammation, cardiovascular dysfunction, and endocrine-metabolic imbalance, referred to as allostatic load [67, 68].

Our study extends the limited body of evidence on the relationship between food environments and cognitive function in US older adults. A previous longitudinal study found inconsistent findings, suggesting that residing a neighborhood with more grocery stores (number per 1000 population in a census tract) was not associated with cognitive function score based on five cognitive tests among 221,151 Black and White Americans aged 45 or older residing in metropolitan areas [33]. The discrepancy may come from differences in sample characteristics, such as age distribution and racialized group composition because the current study was conducted in a representative sample of Medicare beneficiaries aged 65 and older. Importantly, the measure of the food environment differs: the previous study used grocery store density within a census tract, while our current study assessed low food access in terms of proximity and its combination with low-income within a census tract. Accounting for both distance to healthy food stores and the neighborhood income level may better capture the risk factors of cognitive decline that older adults face in daily lives in urban neighborhoods. This also implies the importance of considering not only access to healthy food outlets but also other contextual factors to improve healthy eating and cognitive function. Addressing only one of these factors might not be enough.

Individuals with low-income and those racialized as Black or Latinx often experience more modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline, such as physical inactivity [69, 70]. Our analysis showed that these individuals are more likely to live in neighborhoods with both low food access and low- income. Although individual factors are strong predictors of cognitive decline, structural factor—low food access and low-income neighborhoods—is independently associated with accelerated cognitive decline, even after controlling for individual-level sociodemographic factors such as income and educational attainment. Living in a neighborhood with both poor food access and low-income may create a double disadvantage by compounding individual risk factors and exacerbating disparities in diet quality, food insecurity, and cognitive decline, particularly along the lines of income, race, and ethnicity. For example, low-income households living in a neighborhood with both low access and low-income may face additional, yet avoidable, structural barriers, such as increased travel time to obtain affordable and healthy food—further burdening those already experiencing financial hardship on a limited budget [71, 72]. Addressing only individual factors may not be sufficient to reduce nutrition inequities and disparities in cognitive decline.

The association between residing in neighborhoods with both low food access and low-income and cognitive decline underscores the need to consider environmental factors to improve cognitive health outcomes for community-dwelling older adults in urban areas. For example, individual-level dietary and behavioral interventions that encourage healthy food purchasing and consumption should also assess and address participants’ food environments including proximity to healthy food stores, availability of affordable healthy food options, and access to transportation to these food outlets. Additionally, community gardens which increase access to fruits and vegetables, physical activity, and social connections [73] can be potential, environmental-level strategies in urban low food access and low-income areas. Lastly, participation in the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP) has been associated with slower cognitive decline compared to non-participants [74, 75]. Policies that require SNAP retailers to stock a variety of food choices could also help increase access to healthy food options in low-income communities, thereby improving cognitive function.

This study has several limitations that should be considered when interpreting the findings. First, it is possible that people living in low food access and low-income neighborhoods at baseline may move to other locations and this can result in misclassification of exposure status. Moreover, even if individuals remain in the same location, the food environment may change over time. We explored if living in neighborhoods with both low access and low-income was associated with residential mobility during study years in our sample; adding residential mobility into the final models did not alter the inference for the hypothesized association. We also found that racially and ethnically minoritized individuals, as well as those with lower income and lower educational attainment, are more likely to reside in low food access and low-income areas. This is especially relevant for older adults, who often rely on fixed income sources. As a result, even when they relocate, they are likely to move to neighborhoods with similar income levels. These patterns may not simply reflect personal choice but rather constrained or forced decisions shaped by structural factors. Given this, we expect that these individuals may have lived in such neighborhoods not only at baseline but also in the past or during the follow-up period. As a result, the true association may be stronger when capturing the cumulative impacts of residing in low food access and low-income area, rather than assessing exposure only at baseline. Future studies should consider capturing the duration of living in low food access and low-income neighborhoods in relation to cognitive decline to better understand its accumulative impacts. Second, food purchasing and dietary habits are affected by multiple factors such as physical and social environmental characteristics, culture, and religion [76, 77], but in this study, we captured only part of food environments. Future studies should consider dynamic food systems consisting of multiple factors that affect people’s purchasing and dietary behaviors, such as food prices, opening hours, reliable transportation to food stores, social capital, neighborhood safety, culturally tailored mobile markets in multiethnic communities, and food banks to better quantify impact of food environments on healthy diets and cognitive decline. Third, despite adjusting for both individual- and neighborhood-level potential confounders, residual confounding due to unmeasured factors such as wealth and employment cannot be ruled out. However, we controlled for available socioeconomic factors such as education and income, which are strong predictors of other unmeasured individual- and neighborhood-level confounders or mediators that may be involved in causal pathways. Therefore, our estimated association is likely underestimated toward the null. Lastly, although NHATS oversampled Black individuals and we applied survey weights to enhance generalizability, Medicare beneficiaries may still not be fully representative of the US older adult population, particularly in terms of income. However, NHATS data closely align with US Census Bureau data in terms of age groups and sex when survey weights are applied [36].

Conclusions

This study provides new knowledge highlighting the importance of neighborhood environments in which people live their daily lives for cognitive decline. The findings suggest that residing in a neighborhood with low food access and low-income is associated with an accelerated cognitive decline among urban-dwelling older adults. These findings are notable because people who are Black, Indigenous, and Latinx are likely to live in these unhealthy food environments [7, 78] and the negative association of the food environments with cognitive declines can function as additional environmental hazards for the people who are experiencing health disparities and contribute to widening the health disparities. The study findings can be helpful to develop strategies to reduce food access inequities and cognitive health disparities. Policy makers or advocates can consider increasing food access and reducing poverty as top-down intervention strategies to improve cognitive health among older populations, particularly those living in urban areas.

Data availability

Food Access Research Atlas data (https://www.ers.usda.gov/data-products/food-access-research-atlas/download-the-data/) and National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) data (https://www.nhats.org/researcher/nhats) are publicly available. However, the Census tract codes for NHATS participants are restricted data and cannot be publicly shared.

Abbreviations

US:

United States

NHATS:

National Health and Aging Trends Study

USDA:

United States Department of Agriculture

SNAP:

Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program

SD:

Standard deviation

CI:

Confidence interval

References

  1. Caspi CE, Sorensen G, Subramanian SV, Kawachi I. The local food environment and diet: a systematic review. Health Place. 2012;18(5):1172–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Turner C, Aggarwal A, Walls H, Herforth A, Drewnowski A, Coates J, et al. Concepts and critical perspectives for food environment research: a global framework with implications for action in low- and middle-income countries. Glob Food Sec. 2018;18:93–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Brown DR, Brewster LG. The food environment is a complex social network. Soc Sci Med. 2015;133:202–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Walker RE, Keane CR, Burke JG. Disparities and access to healthy food in the United States: a review of food deserts literature. Health Place. 2010;16(5):876–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Papanek A, Campbell CG, Shelnutt K. Disparities in community food environments: FCS3381/FY1520, 5/2023. EDIS. 2023;2023(3):1–6. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.32473/edis-fy1520-2023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Li M, Yuan F. Historical redlining and food environments: a study of 102 urban areas in the United States. Health Place. 2022;75: 102775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bower KM, Thorpe RJ Jr, Rohde C, Gaskin DJ. The intersection of neighborhood racial segregation, poverty, and urbanicity and its impact on food store availability in the United States. Prev Med. 2014;58:33–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Franco M, Diez Roux AV, Glass TA, Caballero B, Brancati FL. Neighborhood characteristics and availability of healthy foods in Baltimore. Am J Prev Med. 2008;35(6):561–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Krukowski RA, West DS, Harvey-Berino J, Elaine PT. Neighborhood impact on healthy food availability and pricing in food stores. J Community Health. 2010;35(3):315–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Andreyeva T, Blumenthal DM, Schwartz MB, Long MW, Brownell KD. Availability and prices of foods across stores and neighborhoods: the case of New Haven. Connecticut Health Aff (Millwood). 2008;27(5):1381–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Gosliner W, Brown DM, Sun BC, Woodward-Lopez G, Crawford PB. Availability, quality and price of produce in low-income neighbourhood food stores in California raise equity issues. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21(9):1639–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Cavanaugh E, Mallya G, Brensinger C, Tierney A, Glanz K. Nutrition environments in corner stores in Philadelphia. Prev Med. 2013;56(2):149–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Hurvitz PM, Moudon AV, Rehm CD, Streichert LC, Drewnowski A. Arterial roads and area socioeconomic status are predictors of fast food restaurant density in King County, WA. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2009;6:46.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Moore LV, Diez Roux AV. Associations of neighborhood characteristics with the location and type of food stores. Am J Public Health. 2006;96(2):325–31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Zenk SN, Schulz AJ, Israel BA, James SA, Bao S, Wilson ML. Neighborhood racial composition, neighborhood poverty, and the spatial accessibility of supermarkets in metropolitan Detroit. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(4):660–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Payton NM, Marseglia A, Grande G, Fratiglioni L, Kivipelto M, Bäckman L, et al. Trajectories of cognitive decline and dementia development: a 12-year longitudinal study. Alzheimers Dement. 2023;19(3):857–67. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/alz.12704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Connors MH, Ames D, Woodward M, Brodaty H. Mild cognitive impairment and driving cessation: a 3-year longitudinal study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2017;44(1–2):63–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Blazer DG, Yaffe K, Karlawish J. Cognitive aging: a report from the Institute of Medicine. JAMA. 2015;313(21):2121–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Kim MJ, Bronas UG, Quinn L, Sharp LK, Park C, Gruss V, et al. Cognitive function and self-management behaviors in older adults with type 2 diabetes. Nurs Res. 2023;72(1):38–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Bader MD, Purciel M, Yousefzadeh P, Neckerman KM. Disparities in neighborhood food environments: implications of measurement strategies. Econ Geogr. 2010;86(4):409–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Byker Shanks C, Haack S, Tarabochia D, Bates K, Christenson L. Factors influencing food choices among older adults in the rural western USA. J Community Health. 2017;42(3):511–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. McCullough ML, Chantaprasopsuk S, Islami F, Rees-Punia E, Um CY, Wang Y, et al. Association of socioeconomic and geographic factors with diet quality in US adults. JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(6): e2216406.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Sharkey JR, Johnson CM, Dean WR. Food access and perceptions of the community and household food environment as correlates of fruit and vegetable intake among rural seniors. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Gregory CA, Coleman-Jensen A. Do high food prices increase food insecurity in the United States? Appl Econ Perspect Policy. 2013;35(4):679–707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Cheung ESL. Food insecurity among older adults in New York City: does location matter? J Hunger Environ Nutr. 2024;19(4):540–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Briggs AC, Black AW, Lucas FL, Siewers AE, Fairfield KM. Association between the food and physical activity environment, obesity, and cardiovascular health across Maine counties. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Mezuk B, Li X, Cederin K, Rice K, Sundquist J, Sundquist K. Beyond access: characteristics of the food environment and risk of diabetes. Am J Epidemiol. 2016;183(12):1129–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Suarez JJ, Isakova T, Anderson CA, Boulware LE, Wolf M, Scialla JJ. Food access, chronic kidney disease, and hypertension in the U.S. Am J Prev Med.. 2015;49(6):912–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Royer MF, Guerithault N, Braden BB, Laska MN, Bruening M. Food insecurity is associated with cognitive function: a systematic review of findings across the life course. Int J Transl Med. 2021;1(3):205–22.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Seligman HK, Bindman AB, Vittinghoff E, Kanaya AM, Kushel MB. Food insecurity is associated with diabetes mellitus: results from the National Health Examination and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 1999–2002. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(7):1018–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Kim B, Samuel LJ, Thorpe RJ Jr, Crews DC, Szanton SL. Food insecurity and cognitive trajectories in community-dwelling Medicare beneficiaries 65 years and older. JAMA Netw Open. 2023;6(3): e234674.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Finlay J, Esposito M, Tang S, Gomez-Lopez I, Sylvers D, Judd S, et al. Fast-food for thought: retail food environments as resources for cognitive health and wellbeing among aging Americans? Health Place. 2020;64: 102379.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Finlay J, Esposito M, Langa KM, Judd S, Clarke P. Cognability: an ecological theory of neighborhoods and cognitive aging. Soc Sci Med. 2022;309: 115220.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Economic Research Service. Food Access Research Atlas. U.S. Department of Agriculture; 2010. https://www.ers.usda.gov/data-products/food-access-research-atlas/download-the-data. Accessed 1 June 2023.

  35. National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS). Round 1–11 public use data files. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University School of Public Health; 2022. https://www.nhats.org/researcher/nhats. Accessed 1 June 2023.

  36. Freedman VA, Kasper JD. Cohort profile: the National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS). Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48(4):1044–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. U.S. Department of Agriculture. Food Access Research Atlas definitions. 2022. https://www.ers.usda.gov/data-products/food-access-research-atlas/documentation/#definitions. Accessed 28 June 2023.

  38. Vicki A. Freedman JAS, Maureen E. Skehan, and Judith D. Kasper. National Health and Aging Trends Study user guide: rounds 1–11 final release. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University School of Public Health; 2022. www.NHATS.org. Accessed 21 Feb 2023.

  39. Lacko A, Ng SW, Popkin B. Urban vs. rural socioeconomic differences in the nutritional quality of household packaged food purchases by store type. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(20):7637. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/ijerph17207637.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Shulman KI, Pushkar Gold D, Cohen CA, Zucchero CA. Clock-drawing and dementia in the community: a longitudinal study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1993;8(6):487–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1989;39(9):1159–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Quiñones AR, Chen S, Nagel CL, Botoseneanu A, Allore HG, Newsom JT, et al. Trajectories of cognitive functioning in later life: disparities by race/ethnicity, educational attainment, sex, and multimorbidity combinations. SSM Popul Health. 2022;18: 101084.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Gooch A. Measurements of cognitive skill by survey mode: marginal differences and scaling similarities. Research & Politics. 2015;2(3):2053168015590681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Norbert S, Denise P, Barbel K, Seymour S. Cognition, aging and self-reports. Philadelphia: Psychology Press; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Fitzpatrick K, Greenhalgh-Stanley N, Ver PM. Food deserts and diet-related health outcomes of the elderly. Food Policy. 2019;87: 101747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Liese AD, Hibbert JD, Ma X, Bell BA, Battersby SE. Where are the food deserts? An evaluation of policy-relevant measures of community food access in South Carolina. J Hunger Environ Nutr. 2014;9(1):16–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Ma X, Sharpe PA, Bell BA, Liu J, White K, Liese AD. Food acquisition and shopping patterns among residents of low-income and low-access communities in South Carolina. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2018;118(10):1844–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Beaulac J, Kristjansson E, Cummins S. A systematic review of food deserts, 1966–2007. Prev Chronic Dis. 2009;6(3):A105.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Latham K, Clarke PJ. Neighborhood disorder, perceived social cohesion, and social participation among older Americans: findings from the National Health & Aging Trends Study. J Aging Health. 2018;30(1):3–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Thornton CM, Conway TL, Cain KL, Gavand KA, Saelens BE, Frank LD, et al. Disparities in pedestrian streetscape environments by income and race/ethnicity. SSM Popul Health. 2016;2:206–16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Zaheed AB, Sharifian N, Kraal AZ, Sol K, Hence A, Zahodne LB. Unique effects of perceived neighborhood physical disorder and social cohesion on episodic memory and semantic fluency. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2019;34(8):1346–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Lee H, Waite LJ. Cognition in context: the role of objective and subjective measures of neighborhood and household in cognitive functioning in later life. Gerontologist. 2018;58(1):159–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Montaquila Jill, Freedman Vicki A. aK, Judith D. National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) round 1 income imputation. 2012. https://www.nhats.org/sites/default/files/2021-01/NHATS_Round1_Income_Imputation_11_09_12.pdf. Accessed 1 June 2023.

  54. McCarrey AC, An Y, Kitner-Triolo MH, Ferrucci L, Resnick SM. Sex differences in cognitive trajectories in clinically normal older adults. Psychol Aging. 2016;31(2):166–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Yang YC, Walsh CE, Shartle K, Stebbins RC, Aiello AE, Belsky DW, et al. An early and unequal decline: life course trajectories of cognitive aging in the United States. J Aging Health. 2024;36(3–4):230–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Choi YJ, Crimmins EM, Ailshire JA. Food insecurity, food environments, and disparities in diet quality and obesity in a nationally representative sample of community-dwelling older Americans. Prev Med Rep. 2022;29: 101912.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Fitzpatrick K, Greenhalgh-Stanley N, Ver PM. The impact of food deserts on food insufficiency and SNAP participation among the elderly. Am J Agric Econ. 2016;98(1):19–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Muñoz-García MI, Martínez-González MA, Razquin C, Fernández-Matarrubia M, Guillén-Grima F, Toledo E. Exploratory dietary patterns and cognitive function in the “Seguimiento Universidad de Navarra” (SUN) prospective cohort. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2022;76(1):48–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Shakersain B, Santoni G, Larsson SC, Faxén-Irving G, Fastbom J, Fratiglioni L, et al. Prudent diet may attenuate the adverse effects of Western diet on cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12(2):100–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Smyth A, Dehghan M, O’Donnell M, Anderson C, Teo K, Gao P, et al. Healthy eating and reduced risk of cognitive decline: a cohort from 40 countries. Neurology. 2015;84(22):2258–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Jayedi A, Soltani S, Abdolshahi A, Shab-Bidar S. Healthy and unhealthy dietary patterns and the risk of chronic disease: an umbrella review of meta-analyses of prospective cohort studies. Br J Nutr. 2020;124(11):1133–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Xu X, Shi Z, Liu G, Chang D, Inglis SC, Hall JJ, et al. The joint effects of diet and dietary supplements in relation to obesity and cardiovascular disease over a 10-year follow-up: a longitudinal study of 69,990 participants in Australia. Nutrients. 2021;13(3):944. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/nu13030944.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Antal B, McMahon LP, Sultan SF, Lithen A, Wexler DJ, Dickerson B, et al. Type 2 diabetes mellitus accelerates brain aging and cognitive decline: complementary findings from UK Biobank and meta-analyses. Elife. 2022;11:e73138. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.7554/eLife.73138.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Dye L, Boyle NB, Champ C, Lawton C. The relationship between obesity and cognitive health and decline. Proc Nutr Soc. 2017;76(4):443–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Ma Y, Hua R, Yang Z, Zhong B, Yan L, Xie W. Different hypertension thresholds and cognitive decline: a pooled analysis of three ageing cohorts. BMC Med. 2021;19(1):287.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Joshi S, Mooney SJ, Rundle AG, Quinn JW, Beard JR, Cerdá M. Pathways from neighborhood poverty to depression among older adults. Health Place. 2017;43:138–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Pulopulos MM, Hidalgo V, Almela M, Puig-Perez S, Villada C, Salvador A. Hair cortisol and cognitive performance in healthy older people. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;44:100–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Guidi J, Lucente M, Sonino N, Fava GA. Allostatic load and its impact on health: a systematic review. Psychother Psychosom. 2021;90(1):11–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Hawes AM, Smith GS, McGinty E, Bell C, Bower K, LaVeist TA, et al. Disentangling race, poverty, and place in disparities in physical activity. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(7):1193. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/ijerph16071193.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Prevalence of regular physical activity among adults--United States, 2001 and 2005. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2007;56(46):1209–12.

  71. Evans A, Banks K, Jennings R, Nehme E, Nemec C, Sharma S, et al. Increasing access to healthful foods: a qualitative study with residents of low-income communities. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12(1):S5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Sansom G, Hannibal B. Disparate access to nutritional food; place, race and equity in the United States. BMC Nutrition. 2021;7(1):29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Al-Delaimy WK, Webb M. Community gardens as environmental health interventions: benefits versus potential risks. Curr Environ Health Rep. 2017;4(2):252–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Lu P, Kezios K, Lee J, Calonico S, Wimer C. Association between Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program use and memory decline: findings from the Health and Retirement Study. Neurology. 2023;100(6):e595–602.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Na M, Dou N, Brown MJ, Chen-Edinboro LP, Anderson LR, Wennberg A. Food insufficiency, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP) status, and 9-year trajectory of cognitive function in older adults: the longitudinal National Health and Aging Trends Study, 2012–2020. J Nutr. 2023;153(1):312–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Brenner AB, Clarke PJ. Difficulty and independence in shopping among older Americans: more than just leaving the house. Disabil Rehabil. 2019;41(2):191–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Lillekroken D, Bye A, Halvorsrud L, Terragni L, Debesay J. Food for soul-older immigrants’ food habits and meal preferences after immigration: a systematic literature review. J Immigr Minor Health. 2024;26(4):775–805.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Bennion N, Redelfs AH, Spruance L, Benally S, Sloan-Aagard C. Driving distance and food accessibility: a geospatial analysis of the food environment in the Navajo Nation and border towns. Front Nutr. 2022;9: 904119.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The National Health and Aging Trends Study was produced and distributed by www.nhats.org with funding from the National Institute on Aging (grant number U01 AG032947).

Funding

The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging (DP1AG069874, K01AG054751, P30AG059298) and National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (U54MD000214).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

BK designed the study with RT, SS, PAJ, and LS. BK undertook the analysis of the study with RT and LS. BK wrote the paper with assistance from RT, SS, PAJ, and LS. All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Boeun Kim.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This is secondary data analysis. The analysis protocol was reviewed and approved by the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine Institutional Review Board (IRB00373710).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

12916_2025_4091_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional file 1: Tables 1–3. Table 1 Association between low-income neighborhoods and low food access neighborhoods. Table 2 Association between living in low food access and low-income neighborhoods and cognitive function: additive effects model. Table 3 Association between living in low food access and low-income neighborhoods and cognitive function: multiplicative effects model.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, B., Thorpe, R.J., Szanton, S.L. et al. Disparities in neighborhood food environment and cognitive decline among US older adults: a cohort study. BMC Med 23, 259 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12916-025-04091-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12916-025-04091-1

Keywords